Time moves more often in poetry : A comparative corpus study

  1. Illán Castillo, Rosa 1
  2. Cristóbal, Pagán Cánovas 2
  1. 1 Universidad de Murcia
    info

    Universidad de Murcia

    Murcia, España

    ROR https://ror.org/03p3aeb86

  2. 2 Universidad of Murcia
Libro:
Time Representations in the Perspective of Human Creativity
  1. Anna Piata (ed. lit.)
  2. Adriana Gordejuela (ed. lit.)
  3. Daniel Alcaraz Carrión (ed. lit.)

Editorial: John Benjamins

ISBN: 9789027211590 9789027257413

Año de publicación: 2022

Páginas: 41-60

Tipo: Capítulo de Libro

DOI: 10.1075/HCP.75.02ILL SCOPUS: 2-s2.0-85142439937 GOOGLE SCHOLAR lock_openAcceso abierto editor

Resumen

It is assumed that humans use spatial concepts to think and speak about time. In this chapter we analyze the spatialization of time in five different literary and non-literary corpora by looking at the frequency with which time units (minutes, hours, days, weeks, months, years, and centuries) appear as the subject of a motion verb. The results reveal that there is a tendency to spatialize time more often in literature – especially in lyric poetry – than in other non-literary linguistic contexts. This supports the idea that meaning construction cannot be reduced to a direct transfer-based model, but rather requires an elaborate network of conceptual integration that opportunistically – and creatively – adjusts mappings and integrations to goals.

Referencias bibliográficas

  • 10.1111/j.1756-8765.2012.01211.x
  • 10.1098/rstb.2017.0144
  • 10.1609/aimag.v30i3.2254
  • 10.1016/S0010-0277(99)00073-6
  • 10.1075/hcp.24.11cas
  • 10.1016/j.cognition.2007.03.004
  • Cifuentes-Férez, (2008), Motion in English and Spanish: A perspective from cognitive linguistics, typology and psycholinguistics
  • 10.1016/B978-0-12-505850-6.50008-6
  • 10.1515/cogsem.2013.5.12.198
  • Fauconnier, (2002), The way we think: Conceptual blending and the mind’s hidden complexities
  • 10.1017/CBO9780511816802.005
  • 10.1207/s15516709cog0702_3
  • 10.1080/01690960143000317
  • Gibbs, (2006), Embodiment and cognitive science
  • Guyau, (1890/1988), Guyau and the idea of time, pp. 37
  • 10.1016/j.cortex.2007.08.010
  • 10.7208/chicago/9780226177847.001.0001
  • Koestler, (1964), The act of creation
  • 10.1017/CBO9781139173865.013
  • Lakoff, (1980), Metaphors we live by
  • Lakoff, (1999), Philosophy in the flesh
  • Lakoff, (2000), Where mathematics comes from: How the embodied mind brings mathematics into being
  • Levin, (1993), English verb classes and alternations: A preliminary study
  • 10.1016/0885-2014(88)90015-9
  • Mandler, (2004), The foundations of mind: Origins of conceptual thought
  • 10.1017/langcog.2014.14
  • Maturana, (1987), The tree of knowledge
  • Mithen, (1999), Problem-solving and the evolution of human culture
  • 10.1515/jls-2016-0008
  • 10.1177/0961463X18783358
  • 10.1177/0963947012469751
  • Pagán Cánovas, (2018), Expressive minds and artistic creations: Studies in cognitive poetics, pp. 57
  • 10.1075/hcp.55.03pag
  • 10.1037/aca0000024
  • 10.1075/ftl.3
  • Slobin, (1996), Grammatical constructions: Their form and meaning, pp. 195, 10.1093/oso/9780198235392.003.0008
  • Traugott, (1978), Universals of human language, vol. III: Word structure, pp. 371
  • Turner, (1996), The literary mind
  • Turner, (2014), The origin of ideas: Blending, creativity, and the human spark
  • 10.1016/j.cognition.2010.08.001
  • 10.1098/rstb.2009.0025